An infant communicates even before talking begins. A language rich environment in the early years helps promote communication and expressive development in the later years.
AN INFANT CAN COMMUNICATE EVEN BEFORE TALKING BEGINS: A LANGUAGE-RICH ENVIRONMENT IN THE EARLY YEARS HELPS PROMOTE COMMUNICATION AND EXPRESSIVE DEVELOPMENT IN LATER YEARS
Before children say their first words, they begin to communicate through eye contact, facial expressions, gestures, cries and sounds. Researchers have provided evidence that even before infants can speak, caregivers and infants show turn-taking patterns. This vocal interaction in early infancy has been referred to as “
Bateson M. C. (1975). Mother-infant exchanges: the epigenesis of conversational interaction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 263, 101–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1975.tb41575.x
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(1), 3–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00701
In protoconversations, the adult may say something and the infant responds by making eye contact, cooing, smiling, showing lip and tongue movements or waving arms, which then invites a response from the adult. These caregiver-child interactions help build infants’ abilities for further language acquisition. A study showed that maternal interaction with infants as young as two months occurred in various modalities, including gaze and vocalisation. Bateson M. C. (1975). Mother-infant exchanges: the epigenesis of conversational interaction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 263, 101–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1975.tb41575.x Yoo, H., Bowman, D. A., & Oller, D. K. (2018). The Origin of Protoconversation: An Examination of Caregiver Responses to Cry and Speech-Like Vocalizations. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1510. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01510 (Level V) USA.
These “conversation-like” interactions go back and forth between the adult and the infant for several turns. Studies have shown that the timings of these sequences are like that of adult verbal conversations. A study on infants aged between 8 and 21 weeks based on the analysis of 176 samples of naturalistic face-to-face interactions clearly showed that infants can initiate these conversations. Turn-taking in preverbal interaction adapts to infants’ changing motives for communicating and learning. Additionally, this also paves the road for learning the crucial socio-cognitive skills that precede and enable language use.
The early non-cry vocalisations of infants are also salient social signals. Caregivers spontaneously respond to 30-50% of these sounds, and their responsiveness to infants' prelinguistic non-cry vocalisations facilitates the development of phonology (ability to discern word sounds) and speech. ] In an experiment which involved 38 five-month-old infants, it was shown that by 5 months, infants could learn that their prelinguistic vocalisations elicit reactions from others. By this age, vocalising has acquired instrumental value and infants expect even unfamiliar social partners to respond to their vocalisations.[5][
In this experiment, the infants engaged in a two-minute still-face interaction with an unfamiliar adult. When the adult assumed a still face, infants showed a burst of vocalisation to try to get the face to respond (extinction bursts). This pattern of infant vocalisations suggests that five-month-olds have learned the social efficacy of their vocalisations on caregivers' behaviour. Furthermore, the magnitude of five-month infants' extinction bursts predicted their language comprehension at 13 months.
Evidence shows that caregivers' speech to babbling infants provides crucial, real-time guidance to the development of their prelinguistic vocalisations. Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants' babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19(5), 515–523. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02117.x Morgan, L., & Wren, Y. E. (2018). A Systematic Review of the Literature on Early Vocalizations and Babbling Patterns in Young Children. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 40(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740118760215
Some studies have shown that during the transition between the babbling period and first words (i.e. before the infant can produce two joined words, one after the other), the gestural system and system of speech are already closely linked. Researchers found that young children produce the most gestures when they start saying their first words.
After the protoconversation stage, children gradually begin to progress to generate increasingly understandable sounds or verbal communication. Eventually, they demonstrate their expressive language abilities by asking questions and responding to them and repeating of sounds or rhymes. Children typically acquire their first 50 words between the ages of 1 and 2. Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveira, E. (2015). Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1167. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01167 Kuczaj, S. A. (1999). The world of words: Thoughts on the development of a lexicon.In Barrett, M. (Ed.), The development of language (pp. 133-160). Hove: Psychology Press.
For expressive skills and communication to develop, a language-rich environment that builds receptive skills must be established first. National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel, a scientific synthesis of early literacy development and implications for intervention. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy. Strickland, D. S., & Shanahan, T. (2004). Laying the groundwork for literacy. Educational Leadership, 61(6), 74–77.
Researchers carried out a meta-synthesis review of 103 studies from peer reviewed journals to examined children’s exposure to language nutrition during their first 3 years. While initial assessments of the children occurred before the child’s third birthday, outcome measures were obtained during an age range of 6 months to 8 years. The research found that providing “language nutrition”, which incorporates caregiver talk, social interaction and shared reading experiences to which young children are exposed impacted how the infants below 3 acquired language. The research found that the quantity as well as quality of words spoken to a child has a significant impact on a child’s receptive as well as expressive vocabulary.