Exposure to different language sounds in infancy helps the child to develop a bigger vocabulary when the child is older and caregivers can help with language exposure by talking, singing and reading to infants on a face-to-face basis.
EXPOSURE TO DIFFERENT SPEECH SOUNDS IN INFANCY HELPS THE CHILD TO DEVELOP A BIGGER VOCABULARY WHEN THE CHILD IS OLDER
- Honig, A. (Ed.) (2014). Fostering Early Language with Infants and Toddlers. Montessori Life, 26(2), 28–31.
Jusczyk, P. W. (1997). Language, speech, and communication. The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. (Level III)
Kuhl, P. K. (2000). A new view of language acquisition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(22), 11850–11857. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.22.11850
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1999). Influences on infant speech processing: Toward a new synthesis. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 509–535. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.509
Exposing children to different speech sounds when they are young helps them to develop a bigger vocabulary when they are older. 40 typically developing infants (22 girls and 18 boys) participated in a study in Boston and were tracked from 7½ months through 24 months. Results demonstrated a strong degree of association between infant word segmentation abilities at 7 months and productive vocabulary size at 24 months, suggesting that greater word exposure at a younger age could help expand their vocabulary at a later age.
Studies have shown that from as early as infancy, children are able to focus on what adults are talking about and hear the speech sounds used around them.
In a study by researchers, Tsao, Liu, and Kuhl, speech discrimination was measured in 28 six-month-old infants using a conditioned head-turn task. At 13, 16, and 24 months of age, language development was assessed in these same children. Results demonstrated significant correlations between speech perception at 6 months of age and later language (word understanding, word production, phrase understanding).
The finding that speech perception performance at 6 months predicts language at 2 years supports the idea that phonetic perception may play an important role in language acquisition. This suggests parents should interact with and speak to their infants because speech perception from as young as 6 months of age can affect their ability to acquire language when they are 2 years old and in the later years.
Parents who interact with infants and toddlers on a face-to-face basis, through eye gazing and talking about what they both are seeing and experiencing, can help children build attention, language and learning skills. Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science, 8(6), 535–543. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00445.x Feldman H. M. (2019). How Young Children Learn Language and Speech. Pediatrics in Review, 40(8), 398–411. https://doi.org/10.1542/pir.2017-0325 Rudd, L. C., Cain, D. W., & Saxon, T. F. (2008). Does Improving Joint Attention in Low-Quality Child-Care Enhance Language Development, Early Child Development and Care, 178(3), 315–338. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430701536582
Language learning occurs in social contexts where there is active engagement between adults and children.
Children must monitor the input, detecting, for instance, differences between what they heard and what they might have said or what they thought a word meant. Recognition of the discrepancy helps children progress in learning semantics and syntax. Adults can talk about what children are doing or expand on what they just said to facilitate their active engagement.
While the quantity of infants’ talk experience makes up a facilitative aspect of their language environment, interaction quality is also significant. National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel, a scientific synthesis of early literacy development and implications for intervention. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy. Strickland, D. S., & Shanahan, T. (2004). Laying the groundwork for literacy. Educational Leadership, 61(6), 74–77.
A systematic review of 60 studies, also found that when talking to children, parents’ contingency (i.e. talking to the child when the child is paying attention and oriented to hearing and processing what is said) and their efforts in pre-literacy activities (e.g. introducing and reading books with the child) are critical in supporting language development in the first three years of life. Parental elaboration of language such as explaining and expanding on what the child said had little effect in supporting language development as compared to talking and pre-literacy activities. Using gestures and allowing children to interact with siblings and peers also did not have as strong an effect, however, they could be helpful and hence could be used.
Researchers found that talkative parents included affirmative, encouraging, and child-responsive talk into their talk repertoire – features that were much less apparent in homes where infants experienced very low levels of talk.
Talking to children during daily caregiving routines also helps them to acquire listening, comprehension and communication skills. Honig, A. (Ed.) (2014). Fostering Early Language with Infants and Toddlers. Montessori Life, 26(2), 28–31. Girolametto, L., & Weitzman, E. (2002). Responsiveness of Child Care Providers in Interactions With Toddlers and Preschoolers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33(4), 268–281. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2002/022)
Not all kinds of parental input facilitate language development. Research has shown some forms of maternal directives, such as those that facilitate the child's engagement in ongoing activity or interests, have been associated with better child language outcomes. However, directive language that changes the child's focus, or has the primary goal of directing or managing the child's behaviour has been shown to slow down pace of children’s language development because they do not encourage them to respond or contribute verbally.
Calling infants by their names has been found to be helpful in guiding their attention during conversation. Infants are sensitive to their own name by 4 months of age. Infants’ electroencephalogram (EEG) was measured as they heard their own name or stranger's names and while looking at novel objects. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in response to names revealed that infants differentiate their own name from strangers’ names from the first phoneme. The amplitude of the ERPs to objects indicated that infants attended more to objects after hearing their own names compared to another name. Thus, by 5 months of age, infants not only detect their name, but also use it as a social cue to guide their attention to events and objects in the world.
Studies have also found it beneficial to have a
Lawhon, T., & Cobb, J. B. (2002). Routines that build emergent literacy skills in infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. Early Childhood Education Journal, 30(2), 113-118.
Mascarenhas, S. S., Moorakonda, R., Agarwal, P., Lim, S. B., Sensaki, S., Chong, Y. S., Allen, J. C., & Daniel, L. M. (2017). Characteristics and influence of home literacy environment in early childhood-centered literacy orientation. Proceedings of Singapore Healthcare, 26(2), 81–97. https://doi.org/10.1177/2010105816674738
- Kupetz, B. N., & Green, E. J. (1997). Sharing Books with Infants and Toddlers: Facing the Challenges. Young Children, 52(2), 22–27.
- Lawhon, T., & Cobb, J. B. (2002). Routines that build emergent literacy skills in infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. Early Childhood Education Journal, 30(2), 113-118.
- Fletcher, K. L., Cross, J. R., Tanney, A. L., Schneider, M., & Finch, W. H. (2008). Predicting language development in children at risk: The effects of quality and frequency of caregiver reading. Early Education and Development, 19(1), 89–111. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409280701839106
Putting sounds (phonemes, rhymes and words) to music may help enhance young children’s receptive language skills (as they may understand words that they may not yet be able to say). Music gives children easy access to practicing language and deciphering meaning, different sounds and words. Children who can distinguish different sounds and phonemes are more likely to develop stronger literacy skills over time. Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., & Willows, D. M. (2001). Systematic Phonics Instruction Helps Students Learn to Read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s Meta-Analysis. Review of Educational Research, 71(3), 393–447. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/3516004?seq=1 Parlakian, R., & Lerner, C. (2010). Beyond Twinkle, Twinkle: Using Music with Infants and Toddlers. Young Children, 65(2), 14–19. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=EJ898687&site=ehost-live